Hot Hand and Lucky Personality: Could
good luck bring good luck and bad luck really bring bad luck?
To look into luck is to take on one
of the grandest of all the questions: how can we explain what happens to us,
and if we will be winners, losers or somewhere in the middle in relation to love,
work, sports, gambling and life.
The studies show that luck can be
powered by past good or bad luck, personality and, in a higher twist, even our
own ideas and beliefs about luck itself. Lucky streaks are real, but they are
the product of more than just blind
fate. Our ideas about luck influence the way we behave in risky situations.
We really can make our own luck,
though we don’t like to think of ourselves as lucky - a description that
undermines other qualities, like talent and skill-.
Luck can be a force, but it’s one we
interact with, mould and cultivate.
Diverse points of view provide
different perspectives on luck: if a secular man survived because he had a
meeting outside his office at the World
Trade Center
on the morning of 11 September 2001, he might simply have thought that it was a
casual chance in life without assigning a
deeper meaning. A Hindu might conclude he had good karma. A Christian might say
God had looked after him. The mystic could insist he was born under lucky stars
Traditionally, the Chinese think
luck is an inner trait, like intelligence or a cheerful mood, notes Maia Young,
a management expert at the University
of California , Los Angeles . “My mom always used to tell me, ‘You
have a lucky nose’, because its particular shape was a lucky one, according to
Chinese tradition”. But there is another cultural difference in perceptions of
luck. “In Chinese culture,” she says, “hard work can go hand-in-hand with being
lucky. And the belief system accommodates both.”
Nevertheless, Westerners see effort
and good fortune from opposite corners of the ring. We are ambivalent about
luck. We live in a society that is neither random nor entirely meritocratic, that
makes the relationship between “hard work” and “luck” more complex.
To illustrate, when a friend is
admitted into a top law or medical school, we might say: ‘Congratulations!
You’ve persevered. You deserve it.’ Were she not to get in, we would say:
‘Acceptance is arbitrary. Everyone’s qualified these days. It’s a question of luck.’
The scientists have researched luck
often looking to sports –mainly basketball and baseball-, where chance plays a
role in even the most skill-reliant players, and outcomes are easy to measure.
One of the most studied phenomena in
the field is lucky streaks, where players seem to be on fire, so to speak –the
official term for it is “hot hands”.
A landmark paper by Stanford
psychologists, published in 1985, declared that the “hot hand” did not exist
and was instead an illusion born of the deep established tendency to see
patterns in our environments. They called it
‘The Hot-Hand Fallacy’.
However, in 2014 three Harvard University students caused big trouble
for hot-hand deniers. They reasoned that, once a player is hot, he might be encouraged
to take more difficult shots, supporting then the hot-hands effect. (Previous
studies falsely assumed a random assortment of shots by players.) The students
examined in detail a video consisting of 83,000 shot attempts from the 2012-13
US National Basketball Association season, giving them enough information to
assess the difficulty of the shots. First of all, they showed that players who
felt ‘hot’, in fact start taking harder shots. And, after taking in to account the
difficulty of each shot selected, they found a small significant hot-hands
effect – that is, those who did well began to do even better over time.
Around the same time, another group
from the University of California , Berkeley
found that opponents intensify their defence against hot players – thereby acting
against a hot-hands effect. Previous research didn’t have enough data to
account for this sufficiently, and thus interpreted any decline in a hot
player’s performance as evidence that he was not on a streak.
Given the above premises, the Berkeley group decided to
look at the sport of baseball, where there is almost nothing an opposing team can
do to frustrate a hitter on a roll. They were on to something: when they
analysed 12 years of data from Major League Baseball, they found that how a
player performed the most recent 25 times at bat was a significant predictor of
how he would do the next time. They also calculated that a hot player was 30 %
more likely to hit a home run than if he were not on a winning streak. Lucky
streaks are real and not just an illusion, they said.
But what causes them? Is it truly
luck, or something else? Perhaps it is a question of possibilities. That’s the
suggestion from research into both winning and losing streaks from University College
London. Researchers analysed about half a million sports bets (courtesy of an
online gambling company) and found that those on winning streaks were much more
likely than to keep winning, and those on losing streaks were more likely to
keep losing, than a 50/50 chances would suggest.
Then the team dug deeper to expose
why these streaks were in fact real: it was the bettors’ behaviour. As soon as
they realised they were winning, they made safer bets, supposing their streaks
could not last forever. In other words, they did not believe themselves to have
hot hands that would stay hot. A different impulse drove gamblers who lost. They
guessed that lady luck was due for a visit, fell for the gambler’s fallacy and
made riskier bets. As a result, the winners kept winning (even if the amounts
they won were small) and the losers kept losing.
If behaviour influences luck, do
people who think of themselves as lucky behave differently from the rest of us?
A 2009 study assessed whether
students believed in stable luck as a trait they themselves possessed. A relationship
is found between the belief in stable luck (versus fleeting luck) and measures
of achievement and motivation, including whether or not the students persisted
at tasks or chose challenging ones to begin with. Lucky people, it seems, are
more ambitious. Those who believe in stable luck will be more motivated to pick
difficult goals and then stick with them. If they consider luck as a chance, as
something ephemeral, then they can’t rely on it because it comes and goes and
they might be less motivated to face hard and challenging tasks.
These findings fit together with the
work of R. Wiseman, professor of Psychology at the University of Hertfordshire
and author of the book The Luck Factor (2003). He says that the
best way to look at luck is as a stable trait – not one that people are born
with, but one they can cultivate. Wiseman searched for people who considered
themselves consistently very lucky or unlucky by gathering data from 400
subjects. He found that ‘lucky’ people are skilful at creating and noticing
chance opportunities (such as meeting an important businessman at a café),
listen to their intuition, have positive expectations that create
self-fulfilling prophesies, and have a relaxed and resilient attitude about
life’s trials. Poor unlucky souls are more tense and anxious than lucky ones.
Wiseman split the tendencies of the
lucky group into behavioural interventions such as getting people to imagine
how things could have been worse when they were faced with misfortune or, more
generally, asking them to ‘switch their daily routine’. As a result, 80 per
cent of the unlucky group reported that, after just a month, they were happier,
more satisfied with their lives, and yes, luckier.
The more one thinks about luck and
the new insights into it, the more its paradoxes come down. Consider optimism:
it was one of the key qualities of lucky people identified by Wiseman. These
might win at life, but their sunny outlooks could get them in trouble in Las Vegas .
This is exactly what happened to
Archie Karas. Just three weeks after he won $40 million, he lost it all.
His ‘lucky streak’ turned into a net loss of $50. In addition to that, in 2013
Karas was charged with burglary, winning by fraudulent means and cheating at a
blackjack table in Lakeside ,
California . But fortune smiled on
him, slightly: he was placed on probation, escaping a potential three-year
prison sentence.
Things could always be worse.
Scissor Sister
No comments:
Post a Comment